Radanović, Ana

Link to this page

Authority KeyName Variants
orcid::0000-0002-6322-5165
  • Radanović, Ana (3)
Projects

Author's Bibliography

Mother’s discipline practices and social norms of early development

Videnović, Marina; Krstić, Ksenija; Rajić, Milana; Radanović, Ana

(Faculty of Philosophy, Novi Sad, 2023)

TY  - CONF
AU  - Videnović, Marina
AU  - Krstić, Ksenija
AU  - Rajić, Milana
AU  - Radanović, Ana
PY  - 2023
UR  - http://reff.f.bg.ac.rs/handle/123456789/5505
AB  - The Theory of Planned Behavior suggests that parents’ perceived social norms and
expectations regarding corporal punishment (CP) use might be linked to their attitudes
and disciplinary choices. Research supports this prediction as so perceived norms
regarding CP use are associated with parents’ attitudes. The focus of this study is
mothers' discipline practice and its relation to perceived social norms of early childhood
parenting. Other studies from Serbia show the first use of CP is around the child’s
second year. Therefore, early childhood is a high time for prevention attempts toward
changing parents’ attitudes and practices. The sample includes 413 mothers of 18-35
months old children from six Serbian municipalities. This research is a combination of
qualitative and quantitative methodological approach. Mothers have reported which of
13 discipline practices, they used in the last month (for example, explaining, yelling,
spanking, shaking) when children misbehave. Also, after a focus group with parents, we
identified social norms of parenting practice during early development. A ten-items fivedegree scale was constructed to assess mothers’ perceived social norms of other
parents and their agreement with them. Cluster analysis (Ward’s method, squared
Euclidean distance) shows three existing clusters that fit with the data study of
discipline practices. Chi-square analysis was used to define the extracted group. The first
group of mothers used only explanations as discipline practice in the situation of
misbehaving. Most of the respondents in the second group agreed with the statement
that their child is too little for any form of discipline (64% compared to 6-3% in other
groups). Finally, the third group used explanation but also inclined more than others
toward violent verbal, such as yelling (93%) and physical disciplining, such as spanking
(45%). These mothers perceived two descriptive norms as more available in their
surrounding compared to the others: 1. the belief that corporal punishment is necessary
for disciplining two-three years old children (F(2, 392) = 12.47, p = .000, Sheffe test, p <
.01) and 2. The belief that babies should be left to cry out, to avoid spoiling them F(2,
393) = 7.77, p = .000, Sheffe test, p < .01). Participants from the second group have
stronger belifes that the mother's role is mostly to take care of the baby while fathers
should dedicate themselves to work (F(2, 378) = 6.19, p = .002, Sheffe test, p < .01) and
that being a good mother mean being committed completely to a child (F(2, 378) = 6.19,
p = .002, Sheffe test, p < .,01). Despite the body of research pointing to the negative side of CP, our results reveal that a group of mothers start to use this form of discipline
during a child’s early development. Their behaviour is supported by social norms from
the surroundings. The study highlighted that changing toward less violent discipline
practices should include debagging the false beliefs about child-rearing behaviour.
PB  - Faculty of Philosophy, Novi Sad
C3  - Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28
T1  - Mother’s discipline practices and social norms of early development
T1  - Disciplinske prakse majki i društvene norme ranog razvoja
EP  - 77
SP  - 76
UR  - https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5505
ER  - 
@conference{
author = "Videnović, Marina and Krstić, Ksenija and Rajić, Milana and Radanović, Ana",
year = "2023",
abstract = "The Theory of Planned Behavior suggests that parents’ perceived social norms and
expectations regarding corporal punishment (CP) use might be linked to their attitudes
and disciplinary choices. Research supports this prediction as so perceived norms
regarding CP use are associated with parents’ attitudes. The focus of this study is
mothers' discipline practice and its relation to perceived social norms of early childhood
parenting. Other studies from Serbia show the first use of CP is around the child’s
second year. Therefore, early childhood is a high time for prevention attempts toward
changing parents’ attitudes and practices. The sample includes 413 mothers of 18-35
months old children from six Serbian municipalities. This research is a combination of
qualitative and quantitative methodological approach. Mothers have reported which of
13 discipline practices, they used in the last month (for example, explaining, yelling,
spanking, shaking) when children misbehave. Also, after a focus group with parents, we
identified social norms of parenting practice during early development. A ten-items fivedegree scale was constructed to assess mothers’ perceived social norms of other
parents and their agreement with them. Cluster analysis (Ward’s method, squared
Euclidean distance) shows three existing clusters that fit with the data study of
discipline practices. Chi-square analysis was used to define the extracted group. The first
group of mothers used only explanations as discipline practice in the situation of
misbehaving. Most of the respondents in the second group agreed with the statement
that their child is too little for any form of discipline (64% compared to 6-3% in other
groups). Finally, the third group used explanation but also inclined more than others
toward violent verbal, such as yelling (93%) and physical disciplining, such as spanking
(45%). These mothers perceived two descriptive norms as more available in their
surrounding compared to the others: 1. the belief that corporal punishment is necessary
for disciplining two-three years old children (F(2, 392) = 12.47, p = .000, Sheffe test, p <
.01) and 2. The belief that babies should be left to cry out, to avoid spoiling them F(2,
393) = 7.77, p = .000, Sheffe test, p < .01). Participants from the second group have
stronger belifes that the mother's role is mostly to take care of the baby while fathers
should dedicate themselves to work (F(2, 378) = 6.19, p = .002, Sheffe test, p < .01) and
that being a good mother mean being committed completely to a child (F(2, 378) = 6.19,
p = .002, Sheffe test, p < .,01). Despite the body of research pointing to the negative side of CP, our results reveal that a group of mothers start to use this form of discipline
during a child’s early development. Their behaviour is supported by social norms from
the surroundings. The study highlighted that changing toward less violent discipline
practices should include debagging the false beliefs about child-rearing behaviour.",
publisher = "Faculty of Philosophy, Novi Sad",
journal = "Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28",
title = "Mother’s discipline practices and social norms of early development, Disciplinske prakse majki i društvene norme ranog razvoja",
pages = "77-76",
url = "https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5505"
}
Videnović, M., Krstić, K., Rajić, M.,& Radanović, A.. (2023). Mother’s discipline practices and social norms of early development. in Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28
Faculty of Philosophy, Novi Sad., 76-77.
https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5505
Videnović M, Krstić K, Rajić M, Radanović A. Mother’s discipline practices and social norms of early development. in Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28. 2023;:76-77.
https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5505 .
Videnović, Marina, Krstić, Ksenija, Rajić, Milana, Radanović, Ana, "Mother’s discipline practices and social norms of early development" in Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28 (2023):76-77,
https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5505 .

Responsiveness of mothers during play with the child

Krstić, Ksenija; Videnović, Marina; Rajić, Milana; Radanović, Ana

(Faculty of Philosophy, Novi Sad, 2023)

TY  - CONF
AU  - Krstić, Ksenija
AU  - Videnović, Marina
AU  - Rajić, Milana
AU  - Radanović, Ana
PY  - 2023
UR  - http://reff.f.bg.ac.rs/handle/123456789/5504
AB  - Responsive care is the central domain of the Nurturing Care framework (WHO, 2018),
because it permeates all other domains, i.e. different contexts and interactions.
Responsiveness is the basis for recognising and responding to the child's signals - in
relation to the child's health, nutrition, protection, developing trust and quality
relationships with others, emotional well-being and learning opportunities. Results
obtained in experimental studies indicate that a higher level of responsiveness achieves
effects in terms of greater autonomy in problem solving, better speech-language
development, cognitive skills and socioemotional skills. The aim of this research was to
examine self-assessment of mothers' responsive involvement in child care.
Furthermore, we investigated the correlation between mothers' responsiveness and
certain socio-demographic variables. In the research, which is part of a larger study, 1084
mothers of children aged 0 to 36 months from several municipalities in Serbia
participated. To examine responsiveness, the Survey questions on responsive
interaction (F. Aboud, 2021) was used, which measures the responsiveness of parents in
one of 3 different situations: play, reading or bathing. The scale has 14 items for each
situation within 2 dimensions: cooperation and interference in activities with the child
(Cronbach alpha on our sample is .70). The vast majority of mothers reported being
responsive during playtime with their child. On average, the overall measure of
responsiveness on a scale up to 5 was found to be 3.25, indicating relatively high selfassessment by parents regarding their responsiveness during play with their child.
Additionally, the item-level analysis revealed that, for most parents, playing with their
child does not cause significant stress. Also, mothers answer the child's questions during
play (about 80%) and positively comment on the child's behavior during play (over 90%).
However, between 25 and 30% of mothers are instructive and directive, more than
responsive, while playing with their child. For them, playing with the child is a source of
stress because they have too many other obligations or the child does not cooperate in
the play. In other words, they disrupt the child's spontaneous play by insisting that the
child answer a question they ask, determine which toy the child will play with, direct the
child how to play, in most cases or occasionally have to show the child how to play.
There is a relationship between responsiveness and mothers' education, so more educated mothers are also more responsive, while no relationship with the family's
economic status has been established. Analysis of the responses of mothers from
vulnerable groups (Roma subsample) shows a significantly lower responsiveness of
mothers during play with the child.
PB  - Faculty of Philosophy, Novi Sad
C3  - Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28
T1  - Responsiveness of mothers during play with the child
EP  - 74
SP  - 73
UR  - https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5504
ER  - 
@conference{
author = "Krstić, Ksenija and Videnović, Marina and Rajić, Milana and Radanović, Ana",
year = "2023",
abstract = "Responsive care is the central domain of the Nurturing Care framework (WHO, 2018),
because it permeates all other domains, i.e. different contexts and interactions.
Responsiveness is the basis for recognising and responding to the child's signals - in
relation to the child's health, nutrition, protection, developing trust and quality
relationships with others, emotional well-being and learning opportunities. Results
obtained in experimental studies indicate that a higher level of responsiveness achieves
effects in terms of greater autonomy in problem solving, better speech-language
development, cognitive skills and socioemotional skills. The aim of this research was to
examine self-assessment of mothers' responsive involvement in child care.
Furthermore, we investigated the correlation between mothers' responsiveness and
certain socio-demographic variables. In the research, which is part of a larger study, 1084
mothers of children aged 0 to 36 months from several municipalities in Serbia
participated. To examine responsiveness, the Survey questions on responsive
interaction (F. Aboud, 2021) was used, which measures the responsiveness of parents in
one of 3 different situations: play, reading or bathing. The scale has 14 items for each
situation within 2 dimensions: cooperation and interference in activities with the child
(Cronbach alpha on our sample is .70). The vast majority of mothers reported being
responsive during playtime with their child. On average, the overall measure of
responsiveness on a scale up to 5 was found to be 3.25, indicating relatively high selfassessment by parents regarding their responsiveness during play with their child.
Additionally, the item-level analysis revealed that, for most parents, playing with their
child does not cause significant stress. Also, mothers answer the child's questions during
play (about 80%) and positively comment on the child's behavior during play (over 90%).
However, between 25 and 30% of mothers are instructive and directive, more than
responsive, while playing with their child. For them, playing with the child is a source of
stress because they have too many other obligations or the child does not cooperate in
the play. In other words, they disrupt the child's spontaneous play by insisting that the
child answer a question they ask, determine which toy the child will play with, direct the
child how to play, in most cases or occasionally have to show the child how to play.
There is a relationship between responsiveness and mothers' education, so more educated mothers are also more responsive, while no relationship with the family's
economic status has been established. Analysis of the responses of mothers from
vulnerable groups (Roma subsample) shows a significantly lower responsiveness of
mothers during play with the child.",
publisher = "Faculty of Philosophy, Novi Sad",
journal = "Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28",
title = "Responsiveness of mothers during play with the child",
pages = "74-73",
url = "https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5504"
}
Krstić, K., Videnović, M., Rajić, M.,& Radanović, A.. (2023). Responsiveness of mothers during play with the child. in Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28
Faculty of Philosophy, Novi Sad., 73-74.
https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5504
Krstić K, Videnović M, Rajić M, Radanović A. Responsiveness of mothers during play with the child. in Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28. 2023;:73-74.
https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5504 .
Krstić, Ksenija, Videnović, Marina, Rajić, Milana, Radanović, Ana, "Responsiveness of mothers during play with the child" in Book of abstracts, Current Trends in psychology, Faculty of Philosophy in Novi Sad, October 26-28 (2023):73-74,
https://hdl.handle.net/21.15107/rcub_reff_5504 .

Don't Think That Kids Aren't Noticing: Indirect Pathways to Children's Fear of COVID-19

Radanović, Ana; Micić, Isidora; Pavlović, Svetlana; Krstić, Ksenija

(Frontiers Media Sa, Lausanne, 2021)

TY  - JOUR
AU  - Radanović, Ana
AU  - Micić, Isidora
AU  - Pavlović, Svetlana
AU  - Krstić, Ksenija
PY  - 2021
UR  - http://reff.f.bg.ac.rs/handle/123456789/3342
AB  - The present study is couched within Rachman's three-pathway theory of fear acquisition (Rachman, 1977, 1991). Besides the direct contact with the objects of fear, this model also includes two indirect pathways to fear acquisition: negative information transmission and modeling. The study aims to explore the contribution of these three factors to the level of children's fear of COVID-19. The sample consisted of 376 children (59.6% girls), aged 7-19 (M-age = 12.77, SDage = 3.57), and one of their parents (M-age = 42.88, SDage = 6.00). The survey was conducted online during the COVID-19 national state of emergency in the Republic of Serbia. The children assessed their fear of COVID-19, general fearfulness, negative information transmission, and modeling by their parents, as well as the level of exposure to negative information outside their home. The parents assessed their own fear of COVID-19 and trait anxiety. Parents' anxiety, children's age, and children's general fearfulness were used as covariates. The results of our path analysis provide support for Rachman's notion of indirect pathways. The more the parents were afraid of COVID-19, the more they expressed this (either verbally or through their behavior), which in turn led to an increase in the children's fear of COVID-19. Furthermore, children's exposure to negative information related to COVID-19, provided by their teachers and peers or stemming from the media, directly contributed to the level of children's fear. The results of the study emphasize the importance of caregivers' behavior during global health crises and provide some clues as to what caregivers may do to protect their children's mental health in such circumstances.
PB  - Frontiers Media Sa, Lausanne
T2  - Frontiers in Psychology
T1  - Don't Think That Kids Aren't Noticing: Indirect Pathways to Children's Fear of COVID-19
VL  - 12
DO  - 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635952
ER  - 
@article{
author = "Radanović, Ana and Micić, Isidora and Pavlović, Svetlana and Krstić, Ksenija",
year = "2021",
abstract = "The present study is couched within Rachman's three-pathway theory of fear acquisition (Rachman, 1977, 1991). Besides the direct contact with the objects of fear, this model also includes two indirect pathways to fear acquisition: negative information transmission and modeling. The study aims to explore the contribution of these three factors to the level of children's fear of COVID-19. The sample consisted of 376 children (59.6% girls), aged 7-19 (M-age = 12.77, SDage = 3.57), and one of their parents (M-age = 42.88, SDage = 6.00). The survey was conducted online during the COVID-19 national state of emergency in the Republic of Serbia. The children assessed their fear of COVID-19, general fearfulness, negative information transmission, and modeling by their parents, as well as the level of exposure to negative information outside their home. The parents assessed their own fear of COVID-19 and trait anxiety. Parents' anxiety, children's age, and children's general fearfulness were used as covariates. The results of our path analysis provide support for Rachman's notion of indirect pathways. The more the parents were afraid of COVID-19, the more they expressed this (either verbally or through their behavior), which in turn led to an increase in the children's fear of COVID-19. Furthermore, children's exposure to negative information related to COVID-19, provided by their teachers and peers or stemming from the media, directly contributed to the level of children's fear. The results of the study emphasize the importance of caregivers' behavior during global health crises and provide some clues as to what caregivers may do to protect their children's mental health in such circumstances.",
publisher = "Frontiers Media Sa, Lausanne",
journal = "Frontiers in Psychology",
title = "Don't Think That Kids Aren't Noticing: Indirect Pathways to Children's Fear of COVID-19",
volume = "12",
doi = "10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635952"
}
Radanović, A., Micić, I., Pavlović, S.,& Krstić, K.. (2021). Don't Think That Kids Aren't Noticing: Indirect Pathways to Children's Fear of COVID-19. in Frontiers in Psychology
Frontiers Media Sa, Lausanne., 12.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635952
Radanović A, Micić I, Pavlović S, Krstić K. Don't Think That Kids Aren't Noticing: Indirect Pathways to Children's Fear of COVID-19. in Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;12.
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635952 .
Radanović, Ana, Micić, Isidora, Pavlović, Svetlana, Krstić, Ksenija, "Don't Think That Kids Aren't Noticing: Indirect Pathways to Children's Fear of COVID-19" in Frontiers in Psychology, 12 (2021),
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635952 . .
50
22
3
20